Chance News 106: Difference between revisions

From ChanceWiki
Jump to navigation Jump to search
No edit summary
Line 109: Line 109:
'''Discussion'''<br>
'''Discussion'''<br>
How do you think Fox arrived at their "90 percent likely" statement?
How do you think Fox arrived at their "90 percent likely" statement?
==Evaluating health studies==
We all know that as general as statistics is, particular subject matter knowledge and familiarity are all important. Here is a useful link for understanding and critically evaluating health news:
:[http://www.healthnewsreview.org/2015/07/nprs-on-the-media-with-a-skeptics-guide-to-health-newsdiet-fads/ NPR’s On the Media with a skeptic’s guide to health news/diet fads]<br>
:by Gary Schwitzer, HealthNewsReviews.org, 31 July 2015
From which we find a consumer’s handbook for evaluating a health study:
<center>[[File:OnTheMedia healthnews.png | 400px]]</center>
The On the Media web site also contains a [https://www.wnyc.org/widgets/ondemand_player/onthemedia/#file=%2Faudio%2Fxspf%2F520523%2F lengthy radio interview] dealing with most of the above items. 
Submitted by Paul Alper

Revision as of 14:26, 7 August 2015

Quotations

Forsooth

You really are old as you feel
"Some of the subjects 'aged physiologically not at all [while] at the other extreme there were folks aging two to three times as much.'"

in: The Week, July 24, 2015, page 17 (online at theweek.com for subscribers).

Submitted by Chris Andrews



Obamacare.jpg

See amazing comments here, as bloggers try to figure out the arithmetic.

Submitted by Margaret Cibes at the suggestion of Sarah Bedichek

More on the hot hand

In Chance News 105, the last item was titled Does selection bias explain the hot hand?. It described how in their July 6 article, Miller and Sanjurjo assert that to determine the probability of a heads following a heads in a fixed sequence, you may calculate the proportion of times a head is followed by a head for each possible sequence and then compute the average proportion, giving each sequence an equal weighting on the grounds that each possible sequence is equally likely to occur. I agree that each possible sequence is equally likely to occur. But I assert that it is illegitimate to weight each sequence equally because some sequences have more chances for a head to follow a second head than others.

Let us assume, as Miller and Sanjurjo do, that we are considering the 14 possible sequences of four flips containing at least one head in the first three flips. A head is followed by another head in only one of the six sequences (see below) that contain only one head that could be followed by another, making the probability of a head being followed by another 1/6 for this set of six sequences.

TTHT Heads follows heads 0 time
THTT Heads follows heads 0 times
HTTT Heads follows heads 0 times
TTHH Heads follows heads 1 time
THTH Heads follows heads 0 times
HTTH Heads follows heads 0 times

A head is followed by another head six out of 12 times in the six sequences (see below) that contain two heads that could be followed by another head, making the probability of a head being followed by another 6/12 = 1/2 for this set of six sequences.

THHT Heads follows heads 1 time
HTHT Heads follows heads 0 times
HHTT Heads follows heads 1 time
THHH Heads follows heads 2 times
HTHH Heads follows heads 1 time
HHTH Heads follows heads 1 time

A head is followed by another head five out of six times in the two sequences (see below) that contain three heads that could be followed by another head, making the probability of a head being followed by another 5/6 this set of two sequences.

HHHT Heads follows heads 2 times
HHHH Heads follows heads 3 times

An unweighted average of the 14 sequences gives

[(6 × 1/6) + (6 × 1/2) + (2 × 5/6)] / 14 = [17/3] / 14 = 0.405,

which is what Miller and Sanjurjo report. A weighted average of the 14 sequences gives

[(1)(6 × 1/6) + (2)(6 × 1/2) + (3)(2 × 5/6)] / [(1×6) + (2 × 6) + (3 × 2)]
= [1 + 6 + 5] / [6 + 12 + 6] = 12/24 = 0.50.

Using an unweighted average instead of a weighted average is the pattern of reasoning underlying the statistical artifact known as Simpson’s paradox. And as is the case with Simpson’s paradox, it leads to faulty conclusions about how the world works.

Submitted by Jeff Eiseman, University of Massachusetts

Predicting GOP debate participants

Ethan Brown posted this following link on the Isolated Statisticians list:

The first G.O.P. debate: Who’s in, who’s out and the role of chance
by Kevin Quealy and Amanda Cox , "Upshot" blog New York Times, 21 July 2015

Because of the large number of declared candidates (16 and growing at the time of the article), Fox News has limited participation in its August 6 debate to those who meet the following criterion

Must place in the top 10 of an average of the five most recent national polls, as recognized by FOX News leading up to August 4th at 5 PM/ET. Such polling must be conducted by major, nationally recognized organizations that use standard methodological techniques.

But of course, polls are subject to sampling error. The NYT article uses simulation to illustrate how this could affect participation. Supposing the latest polling averages represent the "correct" values, they simulate 5 additional polls (as Ethan noted, this is a bootstrapping approach). The results demonstrate that this can affect who's in and who's out, as well as the location on the stage for those who do make the cut.

The Washington Post maintains a State of the debate widget that updates the current top 10 based on the most recent polling results. For July 30 we see

WP GOP.png

Note that the top figure is not a bar chart; it represents the positions of the candidates on the stage.

Updates

Who gets into the Republican debate: Rounding could decide
by Kevin Quealy, "Upshot" blog, New York Times, 3 August 2015

The article points out that Fox has not specified if or how it will round the polling figures. For example, the 9th, 10th and 11th places as of July 30 (shown in the display above) were Chris Christie (3.3%), John Kasich (2.7%) and Rick Perry (2.3%). These are all within a one percentage point range, but Christie and Kasich currently in in the top 10, but not Perry. More recent data presented in the present article have Christie and Kasich at 3.8% and Perry at 2.8%, a tie figures are rounded to 3%.

Upshot GOP.png

Quealy wonders if Fox might then consider having 11 debaters?

Final update: The NYT reported on August 4 that John Kasich Is In, Rick Perry Is out in first Republican debate. The article quotes a statement from Fox News:

Each poll has a different margin of error, and averaging requires a distinct test of statistical significance. Given the over 2,400 interviews contained within the five polls, from a purely statistical perspective it is at least 90 percent likely that the 10th place Kasich is ahead of the 11th place Perry.”

Discussion
How do you think Fox arrived at their "90 percent likely" statement?

Evaluating health studies

We all know that as general as statistics is, particular subject matter knowledge and familiarity are all important. Here is a useful link for understanding and critically evaluating health news:

NPR’s On the Media with a skeptic’s guide to health news/diet fads
by Gary Schwitzer, HealthNewsReviews.org, 31 July 2015

From which we find a consumer’s handbook for evaluating a health study:

OnTheMedia healthnews.png

The On the Media web site also contains a lengthy radio interview dealing with most of the above items.

Submitted by Paul Alper